Early life predictors of intelligence in young adulthood and middle age (2024)

  • Journal List
  • PLoS One
  • PMC6986721

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Early life predictors of intelligence in young adulthood and middle age (1)

Link to Publisher's site

PLoS One. 2020; 15(1): e0228144.

Published online 2020 Jan 28. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0228144

PMCID: PMC6986721

PMID: 31990952

Trine Flensborg-Madsen, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing,1,2,* Hanne-Lise Falgreen Eriksen, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing,1 and Erik Lykke Mortensen, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing1,2

Umberto Simeoni, Editor

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Data Availability Statement

Abstract

Background

Studies on early predictors of intelligence often focus on single or few predictors and often on childhood intelligence. This study compared the contributions of a broad selection of potential early predictors of intelligence at different adult ages.

Methods

Information on predictors was recorded prospectively in the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort during pregnancy, at delivery, and at 1- and 3-year examinations for children born between 1959–61. Adult intelligence was assessed at three independent follow-ups using three different tests of intelligence: Børge Priens Prøve, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, and Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000R. From a total of 4697 cohort members, three non-overlapping samples were derived.

Results

The included predictors explained between 22.2–24.3% of the variance in adult IQ, with parental socioeconomic status and sex explaining 16.2–17.0%. Other consistent predictors were head circumference at birth, increase in head circumference head during the first three years, and 3-year milestones. Head circumference was the most important anthropometric measure compared to measures of weight and length.

Conclusion

Besides social status and sex, the strongest and most consistent early predictors of adult intelligence were physical or behavioural characteristics that to some extent reflect brain–and cognitive development.

Introduction

Psychometric intelligence or IQ is a major predictor of a broad range of life outcomes such as educational attainment and job performance [1,2] in addition to health outcomes such as morbidity and mortality [35]. Identifying early life determinants of adult intelligence is important, not only for understanding the development of intelligence, but also for interpreting potential mechanisms that might explain associations between intelligence and different life outcomes.

Empirical evidence suggests that especially parental education, parental income, and maternal IQ are important predictors of intelligence. Parental education together with maternal IQ and the child’s sex were found to account for 24% of the variance in IQ at age 5 [6]. Maternal and paternal education together accounted for 19% of the variance in IQ at age 8 [7], while paternal social class at birth explained 9–10% of the variance in IQ at ages 7 to 11 [8]. However, only 7.5% of the variance in IQ at age 14 was explained by family income, parental education and breast feeding [9]. According to Mackintosh, correlations between parents’ socio-economic status and children’s IQ scores are generally between 0.30 to 0.35 [1].

In addition to parental demographic factors and parental intelligence, other characteristics of the family background, including parental age and parity [8,10,11], have been investigated as predictors of intelligence. These factors have, however, been found to be only modestly associated with IQ. Studies related to pregnancy and delivery have often focused on specific samples of children born preterm or with low birth weight [12], but general population samples have also identified significant associations [13]. Postnatal factors of importance for later intelligence have included breastfeeding [14,15], institutional care [16], and physical growth of the child, especially in the first year [17]. Finally, early developmental milestones have been documented in several studies to be associated with adult IQ [18,19]. The levels of significance and strength of these early life factors vary across studies which may reflect study differences in the included predictors and covariates as well differences between study samples. Additionally, the majority of studies on early predictors of intelligence uses measures of IQ in childhood, leaving the long-term predictive value of these predictors uncertain.

The objective of the present study was to conduct a systematic evaluation of a broad selection of both well-established and less well-established predictors of IQ in the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort (CPC). Prospective data were available on a wide range of potential predictors, and this made it possible to estimate the relative contribution of each individual predictor, while taking into account other potential explanatory variables. Additionally, it was possible to derive three non-overlapping study samples with intelligence assessed at three different adult ages (very young adulthood, young adulthood and midlife) and with three different tests of intelligence. Thus, the study will essentially summarize findings from three different studies into one and give an overview of early predictors of intelligence through a substantial part of the adult lifespan.

Materials and methods

The study is based on the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort (CPC), which consists of 9125 children born to 8949 mothers at the Copenhagen University Hospital between October 1959 and December 1961. Admittance was based on area of residence (Copenhagen and surroundings), but some were referred due to obstetrical complications or single mother status. When the cohort was established, data on family background, pregnancy and delivery, postnatal factors, postnatal growth, and developmental milestones were recorded prospectively during pregnancy, at delivery, at a 1-year examination, and at a 3-year examination. Detailed descriptions of this data collection can be found elsewhere [20].

Intelligence scores were available for a total of 4697 individuals with data from at least one of the follow-ups. The sample assessed with the WAIS was smallest while the sample tested with the BPP was largest. Consequently, the final WAIS sample included all eligible cohort members, the final IST-2000R sample included all eligible cohort members who were not in the WAIS sample, and the final BPP sample included all male members of the cohort who were not in the WAIS or IST-2000R sample. Administrators of intelligence tests in adulthood were blind to information from CPC.

Study samples

The WAIS sample consisted of individuals from the CPC who participated in the Prenatal Development Project (PDP) follow-up study [21] conducted in 1982–1994. On the basis of perinatal records, 1575 potential participants were invited to the PDP, and 1155 (73%) completed the Danish version of the original WAIS [22]. Of these, 29 twins were excluded, because data for twin pairs are not statistically independent, and the study sample thus comprised 1126 singletons of whom 568 were men and 558 were women. The mean age at follow-up assessment was 27.7 years (range: 20.4–34.5). The full WAIS, including all 11 subtests, was individually administered by three psychologists. IQ scores were derived from Danish test norms [21]. The mean score was 102.2 (SD = 15.2) with a range of 41–142.

The IST-2000R sample comprised individuals from the CPC who participated in the Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank (CAMB) [23] during the period from 2009 to 2011. From the CPC 5282 individuals were invited to participate, and a total of 1698 (32.1%) completed the Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000R (I-S-T 2000R) [24] (Translated into Danish by Hogrefe Publishers) as part of the clinical CAMB follow-up. Of these, 314 were excluded as they were included in the WAIS sample and 50 were twins; the final sample thus included 1334 singletons of whom 578 were men and 756 were women. The mean age was 50.0 years (range: 48.5–51.4). The I-S-T 2000R is self-administered and consists of three subtests (sentence completion, verbal analogies, and number series), providing a total score ranging from 0 to 59 [25]. The mean score was 28.7 (SD = 10.0) with a range of 4–56.

The BPP sample consisted of men from the CPC who appeared before the draft board. With the exception of individuals suffering from disqualifying diseases and individuals who volunteered for military service at an earlier age, all Danish men are required to appear before the draft board [26]. A total of 3307 men from the CPC completed the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP) as part of the draft board assessment. Of these 485 were included in the WAIS sample and 481 were included in the IST-2000R sample, while 104 were twins; the final sample included 2237 male singletons with a mean age of 19.2 years (range 16.3–26.2). The BPP is a 45-minute group test with four subtests (letter matrices, verbal analogies, number series, and geometric figures) providing a total score ranging from 0 to 78 [27]. The man score was 37.9 (SD = 11.4) with a range of 4–71.

Predictor variables

The following variables were available in the dataset and were considered possible predictors of intelligence; descriptive information on each variable is found in Table 1.

Table 1

Study sample characteristics, Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort and associations with intelligence.

NMean (SD)Rangera BPPra WAISra IST
Family background
Parental socioeconomic status (1 to 8 point scale)38544.2 (1.9)1–80.41***0.37***0.36***
Parity (First born %)468949.5First/Not first0.07**-0.010.01
Maternal age (years)467325.7 (6.6)14–480.10***0.13***0.15***
Paternal age (years)451529.4 (7.7)14–710.06**0.08**0.09**
Maternal height (cm)4577163.0 (6.0)139–1950.11***0.17***0.07*
Maternal BMI (kg/m2)426621.8 (2.9)14.3–47.8-0.04-0.07*0.01
Single mother, prenatally (Yes %)468029.5Yes/No0.11***0.13***-0.14***
Pregnancy and delivery
Sex (Male %)469772.0Male/Female-0.19***0.18***
Pregnancy complications (yes %)469746.0Yes/No-0.04-0.09**-0.06*
Delivery Complications (yes %)469740.0Yes/no0.02-0.030.02
Gestational age (weeks)385239.2 (2.4)27–46.50.0010.01-0.04
Birth weight (grams)46223257 (585)1050–55000.11***0.10**0.10***
Birth length (cm)462151.1 (2.7)38–600.13***0.10***0.13***
Head circumference at birth (cm)430534.6 (1.6)25–410.12***0.10***0.15***
Mother’s smoking in last trimester (Yes %)462149.7Yes/No-0.14***-0.05-0.07**
Mother’s attitude towards pregnancy (wanted %)452644.3Yes/No0.21***0.19***0.14***
Postnatal influences
Breastfeeding (months)39783.1 (2.7)0.3–120.14***0.17***0.10***
Mother’s employment (employed at 1-year %)289813.1Yes/No0.010.050.07*
Lived in full-time institution at some point in first year (Yes %)454110.2Yes/No-0.08***-0.09**-0.04
Daycare institution at some point in the first year (Yes %)450813.7Yes/No0.01-0.05-.0.03
0–1 year growth and behavioural development
Weight increase, first year (kg)33637.3 (1.3)3.5–20.60.010.08*0.06
Length increase, first year (cm)332525.6 (4.0)13–50.5-0.06*-0.03-0.03
Head increase, first year (cm)308112.9 (1.8)5.5–22.7-0.0020.070.05
1-year milestones mean3505-0.06 (0.9)-3.10–3.95-0.09***-0.05-0.07*
1–3 year growth and behavioural development
Weight increase, age 1–3 (kg)21114.6 (1.4)-4.1.3–13.10.12***0.10*0.11*
Length increase, age 1–3 (kg)212920.5 (3.8)0–450.14***0.10**0.08
Head increase, age 1–3 (kg)21373.5 (1.2)0–13.50.08*0.080.01
3-year milestones mean25950.00 (0.9)-3.9–3.6-0.14***-0.10**-0.09*

Open in a separate window

a Pearson correlations were used to evaluate bivariate associations between each predictor and Overall mean of milestones (point-biserial correlation for binary predictors)

*p<0.05;

** p<0.01;

*** p<0.001

Family background

Parental socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed at the 1-year examination and was based on the social grouping of the Centre International de l’Enfance [28], in which a 0–5 point scale is used to score A) the occupation of the breadwinner, B) the way in which the breadwinner earns his/her wages (public relief, daily wage, weekly wage, monthly salary and own business or capital; C) the education of the breadwinner; and D) the characteristics of the living accommodation (its size, the number of persons per room, its position, etc.) [29] When the data were computerized, the original 0–20 point scale was converted to a scale ranging from 1 to 8 (with 8 indicating the highest SES). Parity, maternal age, paternal age, maternal height, maternal BMI and single mother status prenatally were obtained from interviews by one physician, A. L. Villumsen [30] who interviewed all the women. Contact was established prior to delivery for about 67% of the women.

Pregnancy and delivery

Sex of the child, gestational age, birth weight, birth length, and head circumference at birth were obtained from the postnatal usual routine at the hospital. This was also the case for information on pregnancy- and delivery complications which were coded as binary variables for the present analyses. Mother’s smoking in last trimester and her attitude towards pregnancy were obtained from interviews with the mother.

Postnatal influences

Information on breastfeeding, mother’s employment, full-time institutionalization of the child, and daycare institution during the first year were obtained from the 1-year follow-up health examination of the mother and the child.

0–1 year growth and behavioral development

Weight, length, and head increase during the first year of life were calculated based on information from the postnatal and the 1-year follow-up health examinations. The 1-year milestones mean was calculated as the mean age of attaining 12 developmental milestones related to motor development and recorded prospectively during the first year of life by the mother; detailed descriptions of this data collection and the milestones can be found elsewhere [31].

1–3 year growth and behavioral development

Weight, length, and head increase from ages 1–3 years were calculated based on information from the 1-year and the 3-year follow-up. The 3-year milestones mean was calculated as the mean age of attaining 20 milestones within the areas of language, walking, eating, dressing, social interaction, and toilet training achieved between ages 1–3 years and retrospectively recalled by the mother at a 3-year examination; detailed descriptions of this data collection and the milestones can be found elsewhere [32].

Data analysis

To evaluate bivariate associations between each potential predictor and IQ at each follow-up, Pearson correlations were used. The rate of missing data on each potential predictor varied from 0% (sex) to 55.1% (weight increase from ages 1–3 years), and due to missing data, all subsequent analyses were conducted using Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) analyses [33]. In the FIML analyses, we used structural equation modelling facilities of Stata 14 (StataCorp LP, USA) to use all available information on covariates in each particular analysis. Preliminary analyses of the WAIS and IST-2000R samples showed no significant interactions between IQ and any of the included predictors wherefore results are shown for the whole sample and are not stratified by sex.

First, a series of multiple linear regression models were conducted for each of the three IQ outcomes (BPP, WAIS and IST-2000R) (Table 2). For each domain of predictors (‘family background’, ‘pregnancy and delivery’, ‘postnatal influences’, ‘0–1 year growth and milestone development’, and ‘1–3 year growth and milestone development’), the predictive strength of the included variables was evaluated. All models included the two core predictors parental SES and sex of the child because these two variables explained a substantial part of the variance in all three IQ tests. Therefore, they were included to obtain a more realistic picture of the effects of other potential predictors in each domain. To avoid excessive col-linearity with maternal height, maternal BMI was not included in the regression models.

Table 2

Linear regression analysis of selected predictor variables and milestone means.

BPP βWAIS βIST-2000R β
Family backgroundExpl. var = 17.5%Expl. var = 19.1%Expl. var = 16.4%
Increase = 1.2%Increase = 2.1%Increase < 0.001%
Parental socioeconomic status0.39***0.36***0.34***
Sex (men)0.19***0.17***
Parity (first born)0.12***0.050.06*
Maternal age0.030.010.06
Paternal age-0.051-0.05-0.06
Maternal height0.06**0.11***0.01
Maternal BMI-0.01-0.020.03
Single mother, prenatally-0.05*-0.01-0.061
Pregnancy and deliveryExpl. var = 18.2%Expl. var = 18.0%Expl. var = 17.7%
Increase = 2.0%Increase = 1.0%Increase = 1.3%
Parental socioeconomic status0.34***0.33***0.34***
Sex (men)-0.16***0.13***
Pregnancy complications-0.031-0.03-0.03
Delivery Complication0.00-0.03-0.00
Gestational age-0.06*-0.01-0.10**
Birth weight-0.091-0.02-0.05
Birth length0.10*-0.000.09
Head circumference at birth0.11**0.0910.12*
Mother’s smoking in last trimester-0.06**-0.01-0.00
Mother’s attitude towards pregnancy (wanted)0.09***0.061-0.00
Postnatal influencesExpl. var = 17.4%Expl. var = 18.3%Expl. var = 17.0%
Increase = 1.2%Increase = 1.4%Increase = 0.7%
Parental socioeconomic status0.40***0.35***0.36***
Sex (men)-0.18***0.18***
Breastfeeding (months)0.07***0.10***0.06*
Mother’s employment (employed at 1-year)0.030.060.071
Lived in full-time institution at some point in first year-0.02-0.030.02
Daycare institution at some point in the first year0.05*-0.01-0.02
0–1 year growth and behavioural developmentExpl. var = 17.8%Expl. var = 18.6%Expl. var = 18.4%
Increase = 1.5%Increase = 1.6%Increase = 2.1%
Parental socioeconomic status0.40***0.36***0.36***
Sex (men)-0.16***0.16***
Weight increase, first year0.060.11**0.05
Length increase, first year-0.09*-0.11*-0.06
Head increase, first year0.050.060.08*
1-year milestones mean-0.11***-0.05-0.11***
1–3 year growth and behavioural developmentExpl. var = 19.3%Expl. var = 19.8%Expl. var = 18.5%
Increase = 3.1%Increase = 2.8%Increase = 2.1%
Parental socioeconomic status0.38***0.35***0.35***
Sex (men)-0.22***0.20***
Weight increase, age 1–30.030.050.12*
Length increase, age 1–30.0710.04-0.06
Head increase, age 1–30.0510.03-0.03
3-year milestones mean-0.13***-0.14***-0.11**

Open in a separate window

1p<0.10;

*p<0.05;

** p<0.01;

*** p<0.001

Second, predictors with a p-value of 0.10 or below for at least one IQ-measure in the analyses of each predictor domain were included in a full regression model that also included the two core predictors (Table 3). This selection criterion was chosen to avoid excluding marginally significant predictors that could potentially gain significance in a model with predictors from other domains. Standardized regression coefficients are shown for the FIML analyses and so is the variance explained by each of the five domains and the full model (in addition to the variance explained by parental SES and sex). Table 4 presents an overview of the increase in variance explained by each model of predictors in addition to the variance explained by parental SES and sex.

Table 3

Linear regression analysis of selected predictor variables.

(Models include all listed variables).

BPP βWAIS βIST-2000R β
Expl. var = 23.7%Expl. var = 24.3%Expl. var = 22.2%
Increase = 7.5%Increase = 7.3%Increase = 5.9%
Family background
Parental socioeconomic status0.34***0.31***0.31***
Sex (women)-0.15***0.12***
Parity (first born)0.06*0.020.04
Paternal age-0.05*-0.040.00
Maternal height0.010.09**-0.03
Single mother, prenatally-0.02-0.03-0.08*
Pregnancy and delivery
Pregnancy complications-0.02-0.02-0.04
Gestational age-0.06*0.00-0.09*
Birth weight-0.070.01-0.03
Birth length0.10*-0.050.08
Head circumference at birth0.16***0.14**0.17**
Mother’s smoking in last trimester-0.04-0.00-0.04
Mother’s attitude towards pregnancy (wanted)0.09***0.05-0.03
Postnatal influences
Breastfeeding0.040.09**0.02
Mother’s employment (employed at 1-year)0.040.070.07
Daycare institution at some point in the first year0.04-0.04-0.02
0–1 year growth and behavioural development
Weight increase, first year-0.040.06-0.00
Length increase, first year0.04-0.090.01
Head increase, first year0.17***0.18***0.16**
1-year milestones mean-0.06*-0.01-0.08*
1–3 year growth and behavioural development
Weight increase, age 1–3-0.040.010.07
Length increase, age 1–30.11*0.03-0.03
Head increase, age 1–30.11**0.10*0.02
3-year milestones mean-0.11***-0.12***-0.10**

Open in a separate window

Table 4

Variance explained (R2) by models of predictors.

Statistical modelR2
BPPWAISIST-2000R
Parental SES and sex16.2%17.0%16.3%
Increase of explained variance:
- Family background1.2%2.1%<0.001%
- Pregnancy and delivery2.0%1.0%1.3%
- Postnatal influences1.2%1.4%0.7%
- 1-year growth and development1.5%1.6%2.1%
- 3-year growth and development3.1%2.8%2.1%
- Final model7.5%7.3%5.9%

Open in a separate window

Preliminary analyses investigated quadratic terms for all continuous predictors, but found only few significant terms, and the quadratic term was not significant for all three IQ tests for any predictor. Consequently, no quadratic terms were included in the analyses. Analyses of Variance Inflation Factors (VIF) were conducted for the final model. For all three IQ outcomes, the only predictors with VIF>5 were birth weight (5.15–6.44) and birth length (5.14–7.47). According to Danish regulations, the present analyses do not require approval by the scientific ethical committee system.

Results

Sample composition

Table 1 shows that the mean maternal and paternal ages were 25.7 and 29.4 years respectively. At least one pregnancy complication was registered for 46% of the study sample and at least one delivery complication was registered for 40%. Almost half of the mothers smoked in the last trimester and 29.5% were single mothers at delivery. A total of 72% of the the study sample were men because BPP scores were only available for men. The mean birth weight was 3257g with a mean weight increase in the first year of 7.3kg. The majority of mothers (86.9%) were not employed in the first year of the child’s life while 13.7% of the children spent time in a daycare institution at some point during the first year.

Bivariate correlations

Table 1 shows that parental SES and sex had the strongest bivariate correlation with the three IQ-measures. Significant correlations with at least one IQ-measure were observed for 25 of the 29 predictor variables and the following variables showed significant correlations in all three study populations with IQ: Higher maternal age, higher paternal age, larger height of the mother, single mother status, larger birth size (weight, length, and head circumference), mother’s attitude towards pregnancy, longer breastfeeding, higher 3-year weight increase, and faster attainment of milestones in the first three years. In the study sample, the Pearson correlation between WAIS and IST-2000R was 0.79 (n = 314), between WAIS and BPP 0.81 (n = 485), and between IST-2000R and BPP 0.76 (n = 602). The corresponding mean intervals between the assessments were 22.3 years, 8.5 years, and 30.8 years.

Models of predictor domains

In the models analyzing domains of predictors separately for each of the three IQ-measures, several patterns from the bivariate analyses were repeated (Table 2). Thus, variables from all five domains were significantly associated with IQ, with especially parental SES and sex showing significant associations in all analyses. In the domain of ‘family background’, parity (first pregnancy) was associated with significantly higher BPP and IST-2000R scores while maternal height significantly predicted higher IQ on the BPP and WAIS. In the domain of ‘pregnancy and delivery’, lower gestational age and larger head circumference at birth were significantly associated with higher BPP and IST-2000R scores. In addition, birth length and the pregnancy being wanted positively predicted BPP scores while maternal smoking was negatively associated with BPP scores. Among the ‘postnatal influences’ duration of breastfeeding was positively associated with all three tests of intelligence while daycare institution was a significant positive predictor of BPP scores. Several predictors within the domain of ‘0-1-year growth and behavioral development’ were significant, and length increase was significantly negatively associated with IQ on the BPP and WAIS, while weight increase was positively associated with WAIS IQ and head increase with IST-2000R scores. Low mean age of attaining 1-year milestones significantly predicted higher BPP and IST-2000R scores while low mean age of attaining 1-3-year milestones not only significantly predicted higher scores on all three IQ tests, but in fact showed some the highest standardized regression coefficients. The only significant 1–3 year growth measure was weight increase that positively predicted IST-2000R scores.

Final model

The mutually adjusted model with selected predictors included variables from the five domain analyses for each of the three IQ-measures with a p value of <0.10 for at least one of the IQ-measures (see Table 3).

Parental SES was the strongest single predictor of intelligence with coefficients between 0.31–0.34 (p<0.001). Sex was significantly associated with both WAIS and IST-2000R (p<0.001), indicating that men scored higher than women. Head circumference at birth in addition to increase in head size during the first year were significantly associated with all IQ-measures with coefficients in the range of 0.14–0.18 while head increase from 1–3 years only significantly predicted BPP and WAIS scores. Another consistent predictor was age of attaining milestones means, with the mean age of milestones at the 3-year follow-up significantly predicting all IQ scores (coefficient range -0.10 –-0.12) and milestones at the 1-year follow-up predicting BPP and IST 2000R scores. Gestational age was negatively associated with both BPP and IST-2000R scores, while the following predictors were significantly associated with only one IQ-measure: Parity, paternal age, maternal height, single mother status, birth length, mother’s attitude towards pregnancy, duration of breastfeeding and length increase from 1–3 years.

Explained variance

A basic model including only the predictors parental SES and sex explained 16.2%, 17.0% and 16.3% of the variance in BPP, WAIS and IST-2000R respectively. The increase in variance by including predictors in the domains (subtracting the variance of parental SES and sex) ranged between <0.0001% to 2.1%. The largest increase in the amount of variance was generally found for the domain ‘3-year growth and milestone development’, reflecting the strong associations with increase in head size and the 3-year milestone mean.

The final model accounted for 22.1% to 24.3% of the variance in IQ-measures and additional predictors, other than parental SES and sex, together added between 5.9% to 7.5% in explained variance.

Discussion

We were able to explain between 22.2% and 24.3% of the variance in IQ with a final model summarizing the findings in the five predictor domains. As expected, parental SES was a significant and substantial predictor in all regression models and together with sex, this variable explained 16.2–17.0% of the variance in IQ. Factors other than parental SES and sex explained between 5.9% and 7.5%, with the most consistent predictors being head circumference at birth, head increase at the first and third year follow-ups, and 3-year milestones. Head circumference was the most important size–and growth measure compared to measures of weight and length.

When evaluating the results of this study it is important to consider the role of time for both predictors and outcome follow-ups. Thus, in contrast to many studies of family background and perinatal—and early postnatal predictors, the present study included data on physical and behavioural development during the first three years of life. Thus, the final model shows the direct effect of each variable and not any indirect effects mediated by growth and behavioral development during the first three years of life. This may partly explain why some of the early factors that were significant in bivariate and domain analyses did not remain significant in the final model. Examples could be breastfeeding and weight increase that predict behavioural milestones during the first year of life [31].

The remarkable strength of the association between the broad CPC measure of parental SES and adult intelligence has been discussed previously [34]. This covariation may reflect both genetic and environmental factors [1] and in addition, SES may modify the heritability of intelligence and increase the magnitude of individual differences in intelligence [35]. To some extent, effects of parental SES are also likely to be mediated through environmental factors influencing cognitive development. The relatively small decrease in the strength of the association between parental SES and adult intelligence in the final model suggests that only few of such relevant environmental factors were included in the present study.

Sex was included because sex differences on the original version of the WAIS and the IST-2000R have been reported previously [25,36]. These findings were replicated in this study, but they may not only reflect cognitive differences between men and women, but also test construction factors favoring men. In fact, sex differences in favour of girls were observed for a later generation of Danish five-year old children [6].

The other ‘family background’ variables were not consistently associated with offspring intelligence across the three analysis samples. Maternal and paternal age have previously been associated with offspring cognitive development, but a recent study of ½ million Swedish men found no association between paternal age and offspring intelligence [37] and the weak negative association in this study was only significant in the BPP sample. The positive association of maternal height with offspring intelligence may reflect the known association between height and intelligence, but it is not clear why the effect could only be identified in the WAIS sample. This is also the case for the negative association of single mother status with offspring intelligence observed only in the IST-2000R sample.

The binary measures of pregnancy and delivery complications were not significantly associated with offspring intelligence, but this may reflect inclusion of common conditions such as hypertension and the fact that the study samples consisted of individuals able to participate in the follow-ups or to appear before the draft board.

The predictive validity of anthropometric measures in childhood for IQ has often been reported, and we observed consistently significant bivariate correlations for weight, length and circumference at birth. However, in the final model, only head circumference showed significant associations with all three outcomes, most likely reflecting substantial intercorrelations among the three predictors (range 0.78–0.87). Thus, birth weight has previously been shown to predict intelligence across the adult lifespan in the CPC, but in this study, birth weight was categorized into five categories and the models were not adjusted for birth length and head circumference [13]. Birth weight and length reflect fetal body growth while head circumference may be more specifically related to brain size, and according to Deary et al. [38] correlations between head size and intelligence are about 0.20 while correlations between total brain volume and intelligence are found to be in the range 0.30 to 0.40. In our study, head circumference at birth was among the strongest predictors of offspring intelligence, and this should probably be interpreted in the context of associations between head circumference, brain size and intelligence.

The interpretation is corroborated by the results for the growth variables. In the domain analyses, only length increase during the first year showed a consistently significant association with later IQ, but this pattern was clearly different in the full model where head circumference increase at both the 1-year and 3-year follow-ups showed strong associations with offspring intelligence (except for IST-2000R at the three-year follow-up). Effects of postnatal growth have also been emphasized in other studies where early growth has been linked to intelligence later in childhood [17,39,40] and early head circumference growth may be particularly important for behavioral development [41]. The standardized coefficients suggest that growth in head circumference during the first year may be more important than growth during the subsequent years, and indeed, growth in head circumference during the first year may be among the strongest predictors of adult intelligence.

Significant negative correlations of gestational age with adult intelligence were observed in the BPP and IST-2000R samples. In the full study sample, gestational age and birth weight correlated 0.52 and the negative coefficients may reflect collinearity with the fetal growth measures.

Maternal smoking during pregnancy has previously been found to predict offspring intelligence in the CPC [34]. The smoking variable in that study reflected number of cigarettes smoked while our binary measure of maternal smoking showed a significant association with offspring BPP scores in the domain analyses, but no significant associations in the final model.

Among the postnatal influences on adult intelligence, spending time in a full-time institution showed negative bivariate correlations. However, full-time institution lost significance in the domain analyses, and neither day-time institution nor mother’s employment were significant in the final models. A positive association in the CPC between duration of breastfeeding and offspring intelligence has been demonstrated in a previous study [14], and this association was corroborated by significant associations in the domain analyses, but only for the WAIS sample in the full model. This pattern may to some extent reflect mediation through measures of later growth and behavioral development.

Behavioral development as reflected in mean age of attaining milestones was a quite consistent predictor of adult intelligence, although the age of attaining 1–3 year milestones was a somewhat more consistent and stronger predictor than age of attaining 1-year milestones. This is in accordance with previous findings from CPC suggesting that a substantial part of the effect of 1-year milestones is mediated through 3-year milestones [19]. Behavioral development must to some extent reflect cognitive and brain development, and as the child grows older developmental trajectories tend to become more stable. This may explain why age of attaining 3-year milestones was among the strongest predictors of adult intelligence–an interpretation in line with previous CPC studies showing that especially milestones related to language and social interaction is associated with IQ in adulthood [19,32].

Methodological considerations

A considerable strength of this study is that intelligence in adulthood was assessed in three different non-overlapping samples with three different measures of intelligence at different adult ages. This allows stronger conclusions concerning the consistent predictors of adult intelligence, but also raises questions concerning the interpretation of different results for the three study samples.

Differences between the predictors identified in the analyses of the three outcomes may to some extent reflect differences between the three measures of intelligence. The BPP and the IST-2000R include similar subtests requiring verbal reasoning while the WAIS is a much more comprehensive measure of intelligence, including 6 verbal—and 5 performance subtests. Nevertheless, the remarkably high intercorrelations among the three intelligence tests despite the long intervals between the follow-up assessments indicate that differences between the three measures probably only play a minor role. Accordingly, differences in results for the three outcomes may also reflect the changing influence of early life factors on intelligence through the adult life-span. Thus, except for SES and sex, 5.9% of the variance was explained by early life factors on the IST-2000R variance while more than 7% was explained for both the BPP and the WAIS. This difference could reflect that the influence of some early life factors are diluted over the life-span, although, some predictors attained the highest coefficients in the IST-2000R analysis (single mother status, head circumference at birth, mean age of attaining 1-year milestones). Finally, there are differences in mean level and variance of many covariates between the three samples and there could be corresponding differences in intelligence means and variance; these differences may also to some extent explain the different findings between the three samples.

For the WAIS sample and IST-2000R samples, participation may be related to intelligence and thus indirectly to some of the predictors included in the study. To the extent that selective participation has reduced the variance in both intelligence and some of the early predictors, the associations may have been diluted in these samples. However, selection bias is not likely to have affected the results for the BPP, although the BPP sample indirectly reflects selection of participants in the PDP and the CAMB follow-ups to the corresponding study samples. The generalizability of this study should also be considered, as the results are based on a Danish birth cohort from 1959–61 characterized by for example a high proportion of pregnancy- and delivery complications (40–46%) in addition to a high proportion of mothers smoking in last trimester (50%) and being unemployed in the first year of the child’s life (87%).

Our analyses are based on one composite score derived from each test of intelligence. Thus, our conclusions concern general intelligence wherefore the study provides no information on prediction of more specific cognitive abilities. However, our results are likely to apply to specific abilities showing high correlations with general intelligence such as verbal reasoning, vocabulary, and spatial ability [38].

A final limitation of the study should be noted. Previous studies have emphasized the importance of maternal IQ as a core predictor of the child’s IQ [6]. Since this information was not available in the present study, it is likely that inclusion of such a measure would significantly increase the amount of variance explained by the predictors and affect levels of significance in both the domain analyses and the final models. Finally, given the potential importance of proximal factors in the first years of life, the lack of a measure describing the home environment, including parent-child interaction, stimulation, and parenting style, is a genuine limitation.

Conclusion

Taken together, we found in three different and non-overlapping study samples that the early life predictors explained 22.2–24.3% of the variation in adult intelligence. Parental SES (together with offspring sex) was confirmed as a core predictor of IQ, since it was consistently and substantially associated with adult IQ in all three study populations, whereas the other statistically significant predictors only explained a small increase in variance in adult IQ. In addition to emphasizing the importance of including infant SES in studies of early predictors of intelligence, the study also stresses the importance of considering potential residual confounding and indirectly mediated effects when conducting such studies. Thus, while bivariate analyses and some domain analyses replicated findings from previous studies for a variety of significant predictors, several of these were not significant in the final models including variables from all domains (birth weight is an important example). Interpreting studies of early predictors of intelligence should therefore take the included predictors and covariates into account and consider both the measure of intelligence and the timing of assessment of intelligence over the lifespan.

In the final model including the selected predictors from the domain specific analyses, the most consistent predictors were head circumference at birth, head increase in the first year, head increase from age 1 to 3 years, and 3-year milestones. Thus, we conclude that–apart from social status and sex–all the strongest and most consistent early predictors of adult intelligence were physical characteristics (head circumference at birth and growth in head size) and behavioural characteristics (1-year and 3-year behavioural milestones) that to some extent reflect brain- and cognitive development.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the crucial role of AL Villumsen and B Zachau-Christiansen in the establishment of the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort and thank the Steering Committee for permission to conduct this study. We also thank JM Reinisch and colleagues for contributing draft board BPP scores and young adult WAIS scores for the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort. Furthermore, we thank the staff at the Department of Public Health and National Research Center for the Working Environment, who undertook the CAMB data collection. Further thanks to Kirsten Avlund, Helle Bruunsgaard, Nils-Erik Fiehn, Åse Marie Hansen, Poul Holm-Pedersen, Rikke Lund, Erik Lykke Mortensen and Merete Osler, who initiated and established the Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank from 2009 to 2011.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this Work.

Data Availability

Members of the birth cohort on which the present analyses are based were born within a narrow time range in an identified Danish hospital. The study includes data of a sensitive nature which, due to the low frequencies of some exposures, could compromise participant privacy even when anonymised. In accordance with the Act on Processing of Personal Data (Act No. 429 of 31 May 2000) of the Danish Data Protection Agency, data thus cannot be made publicly available due to considerations for privacy and anonymity of the participants. However, an anonymised version of the full data set can be made available upon request and with the permission of the Steering group for Copenhagen Perinal Cohort (kd.hnoiger@rekaB.nyL.refinneJ) to researchers who are qualified to handle confidential information in accordance with the aforementioned Danish Data Protection Agency act. The IST 2000R Data are from the CAMB data collection and request for these data must be approved by the CAMB steering group (kd.uk.dnus@bmac).

References

1. Mackintosh NJ (2011) IQ and human intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]

2. Neisser U, Boodoo G, Bouchard TJ Jr., Boykin AW, Brody Net al. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. Am Psychol. 1996; 51: 77–101. [Google Scholar]

3. Batty GD, Deary IJ, Gottfredson LS. Premorbid (early life) IQ and later mortality risk: Systematic review. Ann Epidemiol.2007; 17: 278–288. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.07.010 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Deary IJ, Weiss A, Batty GD. Intelligence and personality as predictors of illness and death: How researchers in differential psychology and chronic disease epidemiology are collaborating to understand and address health inequalities. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2010; 11: 53–79. 10.1177/1529100610387081 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Mortensen EL, Sorensen HJ, Jensen HH, Reinisch JM, Mednick SA. IQ and mental disorder in young men. Br J Psychiatry. 2005; 187: 407–415. 10.1192/bjp.187.5.407 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Eriksen HLF, Kesmodel US, Underbjerg M, Kilburn TR, Bertrand Jet al. Predictors of Intelligence at the Age of 5: Family, Pregnancy and Birth Characteristics, Postnatal Influences, and Postnatal Growth. Plos One. 2013; 8: 11. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Alati R, Macleod J, Hickman M, Sayal K, MAY Met al. Intrauterine exposure to alcohol and tobacco use and childhood IQ: findings from a parental-offspring comparison within the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatr Res. 2008; 64: 659–666. 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318187cc31 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Lawlor DA, Batty GD, Morton SM, Deary IJ, Macintyre Set al. Early life predictors of childhood intelligence: evidence from the Aberdeen children of the 1950s study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005; 59: 656–663. 10.1136/jech.2004.030205 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Lawlor DA, Najman JM, Batty GD, O’Callaghan MJ, Williams GMet al. Early life predictors of childhood intelligence: findings from the Mater-University study of pregnancy and its outcomes. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2006; 20: 148–162. 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2006.00704.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Gajos JM, Beaver KM. The Role of Paternal Age in the Prediction of Offspring Intelligence. J Genet Psychol. 2017; 178: 319–333. 10.1080/00221325.2017.1377678 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Kristensen P, Bjerkedal T. Explaining the relation between birth order and intelligence. Science. 2007; 316: 1717 10.1126/science.1141493 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

12. Linsell L, Malouf R, Morris J, Kurinczuk JJ, Marlow N. Prognostic Factors for Poor Cognitive Development in Children Born Very Preterm or With Very Low Birth Weight: A Systematic Review. JAMA Pediatr. 2015; 169: 1162–1172. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.2175 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

13. Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Birth Weight and Intelligence in Young Adulthood and Midlife. Pediatrics. 2017; 139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Mortensen EL, Michaelsen KF, Sanders SA, Reinisch JM. The association between duration of breastfeeding and adult intelligence. Jama-Journal of the American Medical Association. 2002; 287: 2365–2371. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Strom M, Mortensen EL, Kesmodel US, Halldorsson T, Olsen Jet al. Is breast feeding associated with offspring IQ at age 5? Findings from prospective cohort: Lifestyle During Pregnancy Study. BMJ Open. 2019; 9: e023134 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023134 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

16. Almas AN, Degnan KA, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. IQ at age 12 following a history of institutional care: Findings from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Dev Psychol. 2016; 52: 1858–1866. 10.1037/dev0000167 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Rudolf MC, Logan S. What is the long term outcome for children who fail to thrive? A systematic review. Arch Dis Child. 2005; 90: 925–931. 10.1136/adc.2004.050179 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Infant developmental milestones and adult intelligence: A 34-year follow-up. Early Hum Dev. 2015; 91: 393–400. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.04.006 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Developmental milestones during the first three years as precursors of adult intelligence. Dev Psychol. 2018; 54: 1434–1444. 10.1037/dev0000545 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Zachau-Christiansen B and Ross EM (1975) Babies: human development during the first year. Oxford: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]

21. Reinisch JM, Mortensen EL, Sanders SA. The Prenatal Development Project. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1993; 87: 54–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Wechsler D (1958) The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: The Williams & Wilkins Company. [Google Scholar]

23. Lund R, Mortensen EL, Christensen U, Bruunsgaard H, Holm-Pedersen Pet al. Cohort Profile: The Copenhagen Aging and Midlife Biobank (CAMB). Int J Epidemiol. 2016; 45: 1044–1053. 10.1093/ije/dyv149 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Amthauer R, Brocke B, Liepman D, Beauducel A (2001) I-S-T 2000 RIntelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000 R. Göttingen: Hogrefe-Verlag GmbH & Co. KG. [Google Scholar]

25. Mortensen EL, Flensborg-Madsen T, Molbo D, fa*gerlund B, Christensen Uet al. The relationship between cognitive ability and demographic factors in late midlife. J Aging Health. 2014; 26: 37–53. 10.1177/0898264313508780 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Teasdale TW, Owen DR. National Secular Trends in Intelligence and Education—A 20-Year Cross-Sectional Study. Nature. 1987; 325: 119–121. [Google Scholar]

27. Teasdale TW. The Danish draft board’s intelligence test, Borge Priens Prove: psychometric properties and research applications through 50 years. Scand J Psychol. 2009; 50: 633–638. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00789.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Graffar M. Social study of samples. Mod Probl Pädiat. 1960; 5: 30–42. [Google Scholar]

29. Zachau-Christiansen B (1972) The influence of Prenatal and Perinatal Factors on Development During the First Year of Life. Helsingør: Poul A. Andersens Forlag. [Google Scholar]

30. Villumsen AL (1970) Environmental factors in congenital malformaitons: a prospective cohort study of 9,006 human pregnancies. Copenhagen: FADL’s Forlag. [Google Scholar]

31. Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Predictors of motor developmental milestones during the first year of life. Eur J Pediatr. 2017; 176: 109–119. 10.1007/s00431-016-2817-4 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

32. Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Associations of Early Developmental Milestones With Adult Intelligence. Child Dev. 2018; 89: 638–648. 10.1111/cdev.12760 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

33. Graham JW. Missing Data Analysis: Making It Work in the Real World. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009; 60: 549–576. 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Mortensen EL, Michaelsen KF, Sanders SA, Reinisch JM. A dose-response relationship between maternal smoking during late pregnancy and adult intelligence in male offspring. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2005; 19: 4–11. 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2004.00622.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

35. Bates TC, Lewis GJ, Weiss A. Childhood socioeconomic status amplifies genetic effects on adult intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2013; 24: 2111–2116. 10.1177/0956797613488394 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Mortensen EL, Hogh P. A gender difference in the association between APOE genotype and age-related cognitive decline. Neurology. 2001; 57: 89–95. 10.1212/wnl.57.1.89 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

37. Myrskyla M, Silventoinen K, Tynelius P, Rasmussen F. Is later better or worse? Association of advanced parental age with offspring cognitive ability among half a million young Swedish men. Am J Epidemiol. 2013; 177: 649–655. 10.1093/aje/kws237 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Deary IJ, Penke L, Johnson W. The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010; 11: 201–211. 10.1038/nrn2793 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Pearce MS, Deary IJ, Young AH, Parker L. Growth in early life and childhood IQ at age 11 years: the Newcastle Thousand Families Study. Int J Epidemiol. 2005; 34: 673–677. 10.1093/ije/dyi038 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

40. Smithers LG, Lynch JW, Yang S, Dahhou M, Kramer MS. Impact of neonatal growth on IQ and behavior at early school age. Pediatrics. 2013; 132: e53–e60. 10.1542/peds.2012-3497 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Dupont C, Castellanos-Ryan N, Seguin JR, Muckle G, Simard MNet al. The Predictive Value of Head Circumference Growth during the First Year of Life on Early Child Traits. Sci Rep. 2018; 8: 9828 10.1038/s41598-018-28165-8 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

Early life predictors of intelligence in young adulthood and middle age (2024)

FAQs

What are predictors of childhood intelligence? ›

A large body of original articles across various types of study populations suggests that parental education, family income, and maternal intelligence are dominant predictors of childhood intelligence.

What is the best predictor of a child's intelligence is there? ›

Parental education together with maternal IQ and the child's sex were found to account for 24% of the variance in IQ at age 5 [6].

What are the biggest predictors of intelligence? ›

The single strongest predictor of a person's IQ is the IQ of his or her mother. Also interesting: However, once you get beyond the school environment, it's not a very reliable predictor of performance. Controlling for other factors, people with high IQs do not have better relationships and better marriages.

How does intelligence change during adulthood? ›

Studies have demonstrated that a person's fluid intelligence tends to decrease starting in their late 20s, while their crystallized intelligence tends to increase as they grow older.

Which skills are the best predictors for intelligence in early childhood? ›

Some studies suggest that early math skills measured in Kindergarten, particularly related to counting and number sense, predict reading, math and science achievement in later years. Children's early non-academic skills, such as social competence and self-regulation, also contribute to school success.

Which factor is associated with intelligence in children and adults? ›

Final answer: The factor associated with intelligence in both children and adults is processing speed, which involves the speed and efficiency of neural transmission, playing a critical role in cognitive performance.

What predicts a child's IQ? ›

Parental education and maternal intelligence are well-known predictors of child IQ. However, the literature regarding other factors that may contribute to individual differences in IQ is inconclusive.

What is the number one predictor of success in children? ›

GRIT: Passion and perseverance for long-term goals. Living life as if it were a marathon, not a sprint. The number one predictor of future success – both academically and professionally.

Does your IQ change from childhood to adulthood? ›

Although IQ can change for an individual, across a population it is well established that IQ is stable across the lifespan. This does NOT mean that our abilities do not change as we age. IQ (as the name indicates) is a QUOTIENT, i.e., your score is calculated relative to persons of your age.

What is the rarest types of intelligence? ›

Intrapersonal Intelligence is a characteristic of someone that is deeply connected with themselves. This type of person is usually more reserved but at the same time commands great admiration from their peers. Among each of the seven types of intelligence, intrapersonal intelligence is considered the rarest.

What causes high intelligence? ›

Scientists don't know exactly what causes someone to be a genius. There is probably a genetic component to your level of intelligence. Certain types of genes influence how much intellectual power you have. Your child's genetic influences affect their motivation, confidence, and other traits.

What is the best indicator of high intelligence? ›

Therefore, Not making the same mistakes twice when allowed to repeat a task after receiving feedback is the best indicator of high intelligence.

How does intelligence develop in middle adulthood? ›

The adult brain seems to be capable of rewiring itself well into middle age, incorporating decades of experiences and behaviors. Research suggests, for example, the middle-aged mind is calmer, less neurotic and better able to sort through social situations. Some middle-agers even have improved cognitive abilities.

At what age is your brain the sharpest? ›

The European study, which was released this week, found evidence that we tend to hit our cognitive maximum around age 35 and remain there until about age 45, at which point a long, slow decline takes hold.

What is Elon Musk's IQ? ›

As mentioned earlier, Elon Musk's IQ score is believed to be between 155 and 160. Above-average IQ scores within this range are only reserved for the "Highly Gifted" IQ classification.

What is a child's intelligence influenced by? ›

Like most aspects of human behavior and cognition, intelligence is a complex trait that is influenced by both genetic and environmental factors. Intelligence is challenging to study, in part because it can be defined and measured in different ways.

What is the biggest predictor of success in children? ›

GRIT: Passion and perseverance for long-term goals. Living life as if it were a marathon, not a sprint. The number one predictor of future success – both academically and professionally. A character trait which statistically trumps traditionally-emphasized factors such as IQ and talent.

What are intelligence tests the best predictor of? ›

To predict school achievement.

IQ tests predict school achievement because they are achievement tests for the knowledge and skills students were supposed to have mastered a few years before.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Melvina Ondricka

Last Updated:

Views: 6222

Rating: 4.8 / 5 (68 voted)

Reviews: 83% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Melvina Ondricka

Birthday: 2000-12-23

Address: Suite 382 139 Shaniqua Locks, Paulaborough, UT 90498

Phone: +636383657021

Job: Dynamic Government Specialist

Hobby: Kite flying, Watching movies, Knitting, Model building, Reading, Wood carving, Paintball

Introduction: My name is Melvina Ondricka, I am a helpful, fancy, friendly, innocent, outstanding, courageous, thoughtful person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.