Evaluating Whether Sight Is the Most Valued Sense (2024)

  • Journal List
  • JAMA Network
  • PMC6777262

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Evaluating Whether Sight Is the Most Valued Sense (1)

JAMA NetworkView Article

JAMA Ophthalmology

JAMA Ophthalmol. 2019 Nov; 137(11): 1317–1320.

Published online 2019 Oct 3. doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2019.3537

PMCID: PMC6777262

PMID: 31580383

Jamie Enoch, MSc,1 Leanne McDonald, MSc,1 Lee Jones, PhD,1 Pete R. Jones, PhD,1 and David P. Crabb, PhDEvaluating Whether Sight Is the Most Valued Sense (2)1

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials

Key Points

Question

Which sense is most valued by the general public in the United Kingdom?

Findings

This cross-sectional online survey found that sight is the most valued sense, followed by hearing. On average, participants would choose 4.6 years of life in perfect health over 10 years of life with complete sight loss, and members of the public valued balance above traditionally recognized senses, such as touch, taste, and smell.

Meaning

This study supports frequent assertions made by practitioners, researchers, and funding agencies that sight is the most valued sense among the general population of the United Kingdom.

Abstract

Importance

Sight is often considered to be the sense most valued by the general public, but there are limited empirical data to support this. This study provides empirical evidence for frequent assertions made by practitioners, researchers, and funding agencies that sight is the most valued sense.

Objective

To determine which senses are rated most valuable by the general public and quantify attitudes toward sight and hearing loss in particular.

Design, Setting, and Participants

This cross-sectional web-based survey was conducted from March to April 2016 through a market research platform and captured a heterogeneous sample of 250 UK adults ages 22 to 80 years recruited in March 2016. The data were analyzed from October to December 2018.

Main Outcomes and Measures

Participants were first asked to rank the 5 traditional senses (sight, hearing, touch, smell, and taste) plus 3 other senses (balance, temperature, and pain) in order of most valuable (8) to least valuable (1). Next, the fear of losing sight and hearing was investigated using a time tradeoff exercise. Participants chose between 10 years without sight/hearing vs varying amounts of perfect health (from 0-10 years).

Results

Of 250 participants, 141 (56.4%) were women and the mean (SD) age was 49.5 (14.6) years. Two hundred twenty participants (88%) ranked sight as their most valuable sense (mean [SD] rating, 7.8 [0.9]; 95% CI, 7.6-7.9). Hearing was ranked second (mean [SD] rating, 6.2 [1.3]; 95% CI 6.1-6.4) and balance third (mean [SD] rating, 4.9 [1.7]; 95% CI, 4.7-5.1). All 3 were ranked above the traditional senses of touch, taste, and smell (F7 = 928.4; P < .001). The time tradeoff exercise indicated that, on average, participants preferred 4.6 years (95% CI, 4.2-5.0) of perfect health over 10 years without sight and 6.8 years (95% CI, 6.5-7.2) of perfect health over 10 years without hearing (mean difference between sight and hearing, 2.2 years; P < .001).

Conclusions and Relevance

In a cross-sectional survey of UK adults from the general public, sight was the most valued sense, followed by hearing. These results suggest that people would on average choose 4.6 years of perfect health over 10 years of life with complete sight loss, although how this generalizes to other parts of the world is unknown.

This cross-sectional study evaluates the ranking of sight among 8 senses among UK adults.

Introduction

Research in the United States has shown that members of the public rate sight loss as a greater concern than loss of memory, loss of speech/hearing, or chronic health conditions, such as HIV/AIDS and heart disease.1 Similarly, a longitudinal clinical study with patients experiencing sensory loss has shown that loss of sight and touch cause the greatest decreases in quality of life before loss of hearing, taste, and smell.2 However, to our knowledge, the relative importance of sight compared with other senses has not been systematically investigated in the general population in a nonclinical setting. We therefore conducted a survey to determine which senses are rated most valuable by a cross-section of the public and quantified attitudes toward sight and hearing loss in particular using a time tradeoff (TTO) exercise.

Methods

This study was approved by the City, University of London School of Health Science’s research ethics committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before enrollment. An online survey was delivered by an independent market research company, FlexMR (FlexMR Ltd), who was contracted by the research team specifically to disseminate the survey and collate responses. FlexMR sampled 250 UK-based adults in March 2016 (median [range] age: 50 years [22-80 years]; 141 women [56.4%]). The participants were selected from a pool of UK-based individuals who had previously registered to receive survey invitations from FlexMR and were entered into a prize draw as compensation. Participants’ sociodemographic characteristics were collected (eTable in the Supplement). The survey was open to individuals who may have had a sensory impairment themselves or within their family.

This study adopted a broader definition of senses than the 5 “traditional” senses defined by Aristotle, also including balance (equilibrioception), pain (nociception), and temperature (thermoception). Participants were first asked to rank 8 senses (sight, hearing, touch, smell, taste, balance, temperature, and pain) in order of most valuable (8) to least valuable (1). Next, the fear of losing sight and hearing was investigated using a TTO exercise.3 Participants chose between 10 years without sight/hearing (life A) vs varying amounts of perfect health in 1-year decrements from 10 to 0 years (life B). This allowed for the calculation of the number of years without sight or hearing that a participant would trade for perfect health over a 10-year period. Differences in years traded between groups were then analyzed using Wilcoxon rank sum and Kruskal-Wallis tests. Statistical analyses were performed using R, version 3.5 (R Foundation), and statistical significance was set at α = .05.

Results

As shown in Figure 1, 220 participants (88%; 95% CI, 82%-94%) ranked sight as their most valuable sense (mean [SD] rank, 7.8 [0.9]; 95% CI, 7.6-7.9). Hearing was ranked as the second-most valuable sense (mean [SD] rank, 6.2 [1.3]; 95% CI, 6.1-6.4) but was rated as significantly less valuable than vision (P < .001). Balance was ranked as the third-most valuable sense (mean [SD] rank, 4.9 [1.7]; 95% CI, 4.7-5.1), above the 3 traditional senses of touch (mean [SD] rating, 4.5 [1.7]), taste (mean [SD] rating, 3.8 [1.8]), and smell (mean [SD] rating, 3.4 [1.7]) (Wilcoxon signed rank test for pairwise comparison between balance and touch, W = 36 333; P = .04 after Bonferroni correction).

Open in a separate window

Figure 1.

Dot Plot Showing Rankings of the 8 Senses Under Investigation

In the TTO exercise, participants, on average, chose 4.6 years (95% CI, 4.2-5.0) of life in perfect health over 10 years of life with complete sight loss and 6.8 years (95% CI, 6.5-7.2) of life in perfect health as an alternative to 10 years of life with complete hearing loss. This means that over a 10-year period, participants were prepared to sacrifice a mean (SD) 5.4 (3.1) years without sight (95% CI, 5.0-5.8) and 3.2 (2.9) years without hearing (95% CI, 2.8–3.5) to remain in perfect health (mean difference between sight and hearing, 2.2 years; P < .001). Thirty-eight participants (15%) chose death (0 years of life B) over 10 years with complete sight loss and 15 participants (6%) chose death over 10 years with complete hearing loss (Figure 2).

Open in a separate window

Figure 2.

Responses to the Time Tradeoff (TTO) Exercise

A, All 250 participants. B, Women only (141 [56.4%]). C, Men only (109 [43.6%]). The higher the bar, the more people opted for that number of years with perfect health over 10 years without sight/hearing. Responses for hearing and sight are represented by dark and light blue bars, respectively.

Men were willing to sacrifice almost 1 year more without sight than women in exchange for perfect health (mean difference, 0.94 years; P = .03) (Figure 2). There were no statistically significant differences in the number of years without sight that participants would give up based on age group (mean [SD] for age 22-40 years, 5.3 [3.1]; mean [SD] for age 41-60 years, 5.3 [3.3]; mean [SD] for age 61-80 years, 5.6 [3.0]; Kruskal-Wallis test, H2 = 0.99; P = .61), family history of sensory impairment (mean [SD] for those with a family history, 4.8 [3.3]; mean [SD] for those without a family history, 5.5 [3.1]; Wilcoxon rank sum test, W = 4491; P = .17), or having a chronic health condition (mean [SD] for those with a chronic health condition, 5.2 [3.2]; mean [SD] for those without a chronic condition, 5.5 [3.1]; Wilcoxon rank sum test, W = 7583.5; P = .48). For hearing, people with a family history of sensory impairment would give up significantly fewer years without hearing than those with no family history of sensory impairment (mean difference, −1.46 years; P < .01). Participants with chronic conditions would give up fewer years without hearing than those with no chronic conditions (mean difference, −0.56 years; P = .04). There were no statistically significant differences in the number of years without hearing that participants would give up based on age (mean [SD] for age 22-40 years, 3.3 [3.0]; mean [SD] for age 41-60 years, 2.9 [2.9]; mean [SD] for age 61-80 years, 3.3 [2.8]; Kruskal-Wallis test, H2 = 1.54; P = .46) or sex (mean [SD] for women, 3.1 [2.9]; mean [SD] for men, 3.3 [2.9]; Wilcoxon rank sum test, W = 7247; P = .44).

Discussion

The results suggest that sight is the most valued sense, followed by hearing. This is consistent with convergent evidence from linguistics, showing that words associated with vision dominate the English lexicon.4 Balance was also ranked highly as the third most important sense ahead of touch, taste, and smell. This is consistent with empirical evidence showing that balance impairments and their association with mobility and daily activities can be a key factor in reduced quality of life.5 This further shows that the 5 traditional Aristotelian senses do not comprise the most valued senses.

The TTO exercise showed that people would choose an average of 4.6 years of perfect health over 10 years without sight and 6.8 years of perfect health over 10 years without hearing. Men opted to sacrifice almost 1 year more without sight than women. Interestingly, men’s higher valuation of sight in this study does not reflect real-world health care–seeking behavior, with evidence that men are more likely than women to risk neglecting their eye health. For example, individuals presenting with late-stage glaucoma are 16% more likely to be male.6

Regarding hearing loss, those with chronic conditions would give up fewer years without hearing than those with no chronic condition. This aligns with theories on “focusing effects” whereby healthy individuals, when asked to focus on the perceived effect of a disease or disability, frequently overestimate its negative association with their quality of life and disregard the many other contextual factors which could mitigate its effect, such as the possibilities of adaptation.7 However, a similar pattern was not found for sight loss; those with chronic conditions sacrificed only marginally fewer years without sight, demonstrating that sight is valued similarly highly by those with and without existing chronic health conditions.

Limitations

These results may be specific to the UK population rather than universally applicable. The survey was novel but not formally validated. Moreover, despite efforts to recruit a broadly representative sample of the UK population, we cannot rule out potential bias introduced by drawing on a market research company’s participant pool. Additionally, we did not collect data on the race/ethnicity of the participants.

Furthermore, following long-standing tradition, this study considered the senses as discrete systems. Yet sensory impairments are often comorbid; for example, many forms of eye disease are also associated with hearing loss8 or vestibular dysfunction.9 Future work should consider the public’s concerns around multisensory impairments, ie, by using the sorts of TTOs described in this study (eg, years of sight and hearing loss together traded off against sight and balance loss together). Such research would be valuable given the growing prevalence of multisensory loss in globally aging populations.10

Conclusions

This study, conducted among a cross-section of the UK public, supports the notion that sight is the most highly valued sense, followed by hearing and balance. The study suggests that people would on average choose 4.6 years of perfect health as an alternative to 10 years without sight. Future research could explore how such results generalize to other parts of the world and public perceptions regarding multisensory loss.

Notes

Supplement.

eTable. Characteristics of study participants (N = 250), including years without sight and without hearing sacrificed by group

Click here for additional data file.(49K, pdf)

References

1. Scott AW, Bressler NM, Ffolkes S, Wittenborn JS, Jorkasky J. Public attitudes about eye and vision health. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2016;134(10):1111-1118. doi: 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2016.2627 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Brown GC, Brown MM, Sharma S. The five senses: a patient preference-based comparative analysis. Clin. Res. Ophthalmol.2018;1(1):1-8. https://asclepiusopen.com/clinical-research-in-ophthalmology/volume-1-issue-1/6.pdf. Accessed January 8, 2019. [Google Scholar]

3. Attema AE, Edelaar-Peeters Y, Versteegh MM, Stolk EA. Time trade-off: one methodology, different methods. Eur J Health Econ. 2013;14(1)(suppl 1):S53-S64. doi: 10.1007/s10198-013-0508-x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Winter B, Perlman M, Majid A. Vision dominates in perceptual language: English sensory vocabulary is optimized for usage. Cognition. 2018;179:213-220. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.008 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Maetzler W, Nieuwhof F, Hasmann SE, Bloem BR. Emerging therapies for gait disability and balance impairment: promises and pitfalls. Mov Disord. 2013;28(11):1576-1586. doi: 10.1002/mds.25682 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Crabb DP, Saunders LJ, Edwards LA. Cases of advanced visual field loss at referral to glaucoma clinics—more men than women?Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2017;37(1):82-87. doi: 10.1111/opo.12328 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Dolan P.Thinking about it: thoughts about health and valuing QALYs. Health Econ. 2011;20(12):1407-1416. doi: 10.1002/hec.1679 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Caban AJ, Lee DJ, Gómez-Marín O, Lam BL, Zheng DD. Prevalence of concurrent hearing and visual impairment in US adults: the National Health Interview Survey, 1997-2002. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(11):1940-1942. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.056671 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Willis JR, Vitale SE, Agrawal Y, Ramulu PY. Visual impairment, uncorrected refractive error, and objectively measured balance in the United States. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2013;131(8):1049-1056. doi: 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2013.316 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Saunders GH, Echt KV. An overview of dual sensory impairment in older adults: perspectives for rehabilitation. Trends Amplif. 2007;11(4):243-258. doi: 10.1177/1084713807308365 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Articles from JAMA Ophthalmology are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

Evaluating Whether Sight Is the Most Valued Sense (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Duncan Muller

Last Updated:

Views: 6397

Rating: 4.9 / 5 (59 voted)

Reviews: 82% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Duncan Muller

Birthday: 1997-01-13

Address: Apt. 505 914 Phillip Crossroad, O'Konborough, NV 62411

Phone: +8555305800947

Job: Construction Agent

Hobby: Shopping, Table tennis, Snowboarding, Rafting, Motor sports, Homebrewing, Taxidermy

Introduction: My name is Duncan Muller, I am a enchanting, good, gentle, modern, tasty, nice, elegant person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.