ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation? (2024)

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have distinct core diagnostic criteria but often co-occur. ADHD is characterized by impairing levels of inattention and/or hyperactivity–impulsivity, and ASD is defined by deficits in social communication and interaction, and the presence of restricted, repetitive behaviours, interests or activities1. Since the publication of the DSM-V, both diagnoses should be given when both ASD and ADHD diagnostic criteria are met1. ADHD is the most common comorbid psychiatric condition in referred populations of youths with ASD, with studies reporting comorbidity rates as high as 71%2,3. ASD traits are also common in children and adolescents with ADHD, with 12.4% having an ASD diagnosis4. Consequently, an increasing number of studies are investigating the overlap and distinctiveness between these disorders at the cognitive, clinical and biological level, to determine whether they are sufficiently distinct to be considered separate disorders, or rather represent the extremes of an underlying continuum5. Particularly, the Research Domain Criteria initiative calls for further investigation of transdiagnostic phenotypes and deficits, and their neural underpinnings6.

At the neuropsychological level, there is evidence for inhibition deficits in both ADHD and ASD7,8. Response inhibition is one of the neurocognitive domains most affected in ADHD, in experimental paradigms such as the stop-signal task (SST)8,9, which measures the ability to cancel an ongoing motor response10. This paradigm is unique in allowing the estimation of the stop process covert latency: the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), a sensitive individual index of inhibitory ability10. Nevertheless, previous studies using the SST in ASD populations have reported mixed findings, some showing longer SSRTs in ASD compared with typically developing (TD) children11,12,13,14 and others not15,16,17,18,19. Importantly, comorbid ADHD seems to partly explain inhibitory deficits in individuals with ASD14.

It is still unclear whether these deficits are driven by the same underlying brain mechanisms. A task-specific meta-analysis evidenced reduced activation in bilateral inferior frontal gyri, the right superior frontal gyrus and the right middle frontal gyrus (MFG) in children with ADHD compared with TD children during the SST20. Nevertheless, a recent study evidenced higher activation in a group of non-comorbid medication-naive children with ADHD relative to TD children in regions comprising the fronto-basal ganglio-thalamo-cortical system associated with inhibition21.

In ASD samples, only two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have investigated inhibition-related brain activity using the SST16,17. Gooskens et al.17 found no differences in activation during successful stopping between children with ASD and TD participants. Instead, higher ADHD symptomatology was associated with increased inhibition-related activation in the left and right frontal and middle cingulate regions, linking changes in cognitive control in ASD to the presence of ADHD symptoms. In a pharmacological fMRI study, Chantiluke et al.16 compared inhibition-related brain activation during the SST between non-comorbid ASD, non-comorbid ADHD and TD samples of children and adolescents. In the placebo condition, participants with ASD showed increased activation compared with ADHD in the left middle and inferior frontal cortex (IFC), the left orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), the left superior temporal lobe and the basal ganglia. In addition, opposite disorder-specific brain activation patterns were revealed: there was reduced activation in the left OFC and basal ganglia in the ADHD group, but enhanced activation in the left and right IFC in children with ASD.

It is important to note that fMRI studies using the SST in populations with ADHD and/or ASD vary in their sample characteristics regarding age range, gender, medication history, clinical subtype and/or comorbidities, hindering the characterization of inhibition-related activation patterns in each disorder. In addition, studies differ in their SST design, the within-subject level contrasts, but also the exclusion criteria used based on task performance, hence potentially compromising the validity of the reported results.

To sum up, a growing functional neuroimaging literature has evidenced brain inhibitory dysfunction in both ASD and ADHD. Nevertheless, with only one pharmacological fMRI study directly comparing ASD and ADHD samples16, it is deemed too premature to draw conclusions regarding the shared and disorder-specific brain inhibition-related dysfunctions between these disorders. In the present fMRI study, brain activation during the execution of an SST was explored in a sample of children with ASD or ADHD and in a group of TD children. To capture inhibition ability as accurately as possible, SST performance exclusion criteria and analysis were based on recently provided consensus recommendations by Verbruggen et al.22 on the correct implementation of the task and data analysis.

Based on previous literature, we hypothesized that (1) children with ADHD and ASD would show longer SSRTs compared with TD children, and that children with ADHD would further exhibit longer SSRTs compared with the ASD sample; (2) children with ASD would show higher activation in the left middle/IFC and right IFC, and children with ADHD would show underactivation in a fronto-basal ganglia network compared with TD children.

Materials and methods

Participants

Seventy-four right-handed children aged 8–12 years were enroled in this study. Twenty-nine participants were excluded: 1 participant after the fortuitous discovery of a brain anomaly, 2 participants because of feeling anxious or claustrophobic in the scanner, 18 participants due to inappropriate SST performance and 8 participants for excessive head motion (see below). The final sample consisted of 18 children with ADHD (combined-type), 13 children with ASD and 14 TD children (Table 1). Patients were recruited at the Erasme Hospital Department of Neurology, PsyPluriel-Pastur (European Centre of Psychological Medicine) and autism reference centres at the Queen Fabiola Children’s University Hospital and at the SUSA Foundation (Belgium). TD children had no diagnoses of any psychiatric conditions, did not meet the DSM-V criteria for ADHD or ASD and participated upon announcement or personal query. ADHD and ASD diagnoses were established according to the DSM-V criteria by trained child psychiatrists. ADHD was assessed using the full Kiddie–Sads–Present and Lifetime Version23 criteria for ADHD and the parents of all participants responded to the ADHD Rating Scale-IV24, assessing the severity of ADHD symptoms. ASD was assessed according to the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised, the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule and/or the Childhood Autism Rating Scale25,26,27. Exclusion criteria for all participants were as follows: history of prematurity, neurological disorders, genetic disease, complications during labour with neonatal care unit hospitalization, disabling somatic pathology with a potential psychological impact and contraindications to MRI. All participants had a General Ability Index higher than 70 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children28 and one participant with ASD had a Leiter performance score of 7429,30. All participants with ADHD were medication-naive and did not present any psychiatric comorbidity. In the ASD group, nine participants also had an ADHD-like comorbidity. In addition, only one participant had a history of psychostimulant (methylphenidate) and antipsychotic (risperidone) intake, and another participant was taking antipsychotic medication at the time of the scanning (Abilify).

All participants and their parents gave signed informed consent to participate in this study approved by the Ethics Committee of the ULB-Erasme University Hospital, Brussels, Belgium, and received 50 euros to cover transportation/parking expenses.

Functional MRI paradigm: the stop-signal task

The SST measures the ability to cancel a previously triggered motor response to a go signal, when it is unpredictably followed by a stop signal shortly after. The interval between the go and the stop signals or SSD (Stop Signal Delay) was initially 250 ms, then adapted by 50 ms steps using a horse-race model (i.e., increased when inhibition was successful, decreased when it failed) aimed at an equal distribution of failed and successful inhibition trials (see Fig. 1 for details). SSRT was computed using the integration method22, with SSRT = nth reaction time (RT) on go trials minus mean SSD, where n equals the number of RTs in the RT distribution on go trials multiplied by the overall p(respond|signal). Longer SSRTs indicate poorer response inhibition. Other measures of interest for task performance are reported in Table 1. Based on recent consensus recommendations on SST analysis22,31, participants meeting any of the following criteria were excluded: percentage of successful stop trials lower than 25% or higher than 75%, percentage of correct responses on go trials inferior to 60% and an RT on unsuccessful stop trials numerically longer than RT on go trials.

ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation? (1)

a Participants are instructed to respond as accurately and quickly as possible to go signals, i.e., arrows pointing to the left (n= 100) or to the right (n= 100), with a left or right button press according to the direction of the arrow. Display duration depended on participant’s response speed, lasting a maximum of 500 ms. In 25% of the trials, pseudo-randomly interspersed, a stop stimulus (arrow pointing upwards) was presented shortly after the go signal (20 after a right-oriented go stimulus, 20 after a left-oriented go stimulus) and participants were instructed to answer as fast and accurately as possible to the go signal, and to attempt cancelling their ongoing motor response (i.e., not responding) when the stop signal appeared. They were also encouraged not to wait for the stop signal to appear before responding. All trials included a 1000 ms fixed response interval, followed by a variable interval randomly set between 600 ms and 900 ms. b Initially, the stop stimulus was displayed 250 ms (Stop Signal Delay, SSD) after the go signal. A tracking algorithm adjusted subsequent SSDs trial-by-trial by steps of 50 ms according to the participant’s performance22. If inhibition was successful, then the SSD was made longer by 50 ms to make inhibition of the ongoing response more difficult. If inhibition failed, then the SSD was made shorter by 50 ms to facilitate inhibition. The task involved a total of 200 trials (160 go and 40 stop), requiring a total scan time of ~8 min.

Demographic and behavioural data analysis

Demographic and behavioural data analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 23 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). As part of the demographic parameters and SST behavioural outcome measures were not normally distributed or the assumption of hom*ogeneity of variances was violated, and our sample was relatively small, non-parametric analyses were reported.

fMRI data acquisition

Imaging data were collected using a Discovery MR750w 3.0T scanner (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI, USA) at the UZ Brussel hospital (Belgium). A T2*-weighted spin echo planar imaging sequence was used: repetition time (TR) = 3000 ms, echo time (TE) = 70 ms, flip angle = 90°, matrix size = 128 × 128 × 27 and voxel size = 1.88 × 1.88 × 5 mm3. Two initial dummy scans were acquired and then discarded for the analysis. Twenty-seven slices were acquired in an ascending and interleaved order, covering the whole brain. Anatomical images were obtained using a T1-weigthed sagittal three-dimensional TFE (turbo field echo) sequence: TR = 8.644 ms, TE = 3.244 ms, inversion time = 450 ms, flip angle = 12°, field of view = 240 × 240 mm2, matrix size = 256 × 256 × 128 and voxel size = 0.94 × 0.94 × 1.2 mm3. Stimuli were displayed on an MR-compatible backward projection screen visible to the participant through a mirror in the MR head coil. Stimuli were presented using MATLAB (The MathWorks). Motor responses were recorded using a standard MRI-compatible response device (fORP, Current Designs, USA).

fMRI data analysis

fMRI data were pre-processed and analysed using SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping32) implemented in MATLAB2019a (The MathWorks). Pre-processing steps consisted on slice timing correction, realignment, normalization and smoothing with a Gaussian kernel 8 mm full-width at half maximum. Scan-to-scan motion was assessed using ArtRepair33. Scans with more than 1.0 mm scan-to-scan movement or more than 1.5% deviation from the average global signal were replaced using a linear interpolation of the neighbouring scan values. Images with total movement > 3 mm were repaired. Participants with more than 30% corrected scans were excluded from further analyses.

For each subject, a first-level intra-individual analysis aimed at modelling data to partition observed neurophysiological responses into components of interest, confounds and error using a general linear model (GLM32). To isolate the withdrawal component of inhibition34, a contrast map depicting areas of greater activity on “Successful Stop versus Go” trials was created for each subject. Time and dispersion derivatives were included in the model. Twenty-four nuisance motion regressors were added to the GLMs to further control for movement-related effects in fMRI time series35. Events were modelled at the time of the go stimulus onset with a duration of 1.5 s36. Initial inspection of individual SPM maps revealed no signs of violation of design orthogonality; subsequent calculation indicated that the variance inflation factor was 8.44 for successful stop signal items and 12.48 for successful go items. Individual subject contrasts were entered into a second-level analysis to estimate between-group differences using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). The initial voxel threshold was set to 0.001 uncorrected. For the whole-brain analysis, only clusters with pFWE-corrected < 0.05 are reported, accounting for multiple comparisons. In addition, a region-of-interest (ROI) analysis was performed in a priori determined locations corresponding to regions reported to exhibit abnormal inhibition-related activation in both ASD and ADHD compared with TD participants, or between-disorders differences, i.e., the bilateral IFC, left inferior parietal lobe, left superior temporal lobe, the right anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the right posterior cingulate gyrus, the left MFG, bilateral insula, thalamus, caudate and precuneus16,17,20,21. ROI masks were created using the Anatomical Automatic Labelling toolbox37. Within-mask area inferences were computed using a statistical threshold of pSVC-FWE < 0.05 at the peak level. The p-value was Bonferroni-adjusted for the number of regions examined: pSVC-FWE = 0.05/10 = 0.005. Extraction of β-values and percentage signal change for illustrative purposes was made using RFXplot38. SSRT values (Table 1) were entered as a covariate in the analyses. As lower IQ is a feature in ADHD and ASD making statistical control impossible, and does not meet the requirements for a covariate39, all analyses were conducted without IQ as a covariate.

Results

Demographic data and head motion

Gender was not significantly related to diagnosis and groups did not differ on age (Table 1). IQ was significantly higher in the TD group compared with the ADHD (p = 0.01), which is typical in this population9. Groups differed significantly on the ADHD-RS-IV scores (Total, Inattention and Hyperactivity–Impulsivity). The ADHD group total score was significantly higher compared with that of ASD (p = 0.018) and TD children (p < 0.001) scores. The total score was also significantly higher for the ASD group compared with that of the TD group (p = 0.045), due to the presence of ADHD comorbidity in nine participants (Supplementary Table 1). The inattention score in the TD group was significantly lower compared with that of both the ADHD (p < 0.001) and the ASD groups (p = 0.016). The hyperactivity score was significantly higher in the ADHD group compared with that of the TD group (p < 0.001). Finally, there were no significant differences between groups regarding head motion. Additional analyses including non-comorbid ASD and ASD with ADHD comorbidity separately are reported in the Supplementary Materials.

Stop-signal task performance

Contrary to children with ASD, the ADHD group showed significantly longer SSTRs (p = 0.029), MRTs (p = 0.003) and mean RT on unsuccessful stop trials (p = 0.004), as compared with the TD group. No other significant differences were found for task performance (Table 1).

Within-group brain activations during successful inhibition

The whole-brain analysis of the Successful Stop versus Go contrast disclosed increased activity (pFWE < 0.05) in a cluster comprising the right angular gyrus, the right inferior parietal cortex and the intraparietal sulcus in the ADHD group (Table 2 and Fig. 2a). In the ASD group, the same contrast revealed activation in the right MFG (pFWE < 0.05). For these whole-brain analyses, results were comparable with or without age entered as a covariate. The TD group did not show activation at pFWE < 0.05 (whole-brain analysis).

ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation? (2)

a Within-group activation in a cluster comprising the right angular gyrus/intraparietal sulcus and inferior parietal gyrus in the ADHD group (x = 36, y = −56, z = 34; p < 0.001) and in the right middle frontal gyrus in the ASD group (x = 42, y = 18, z = 46; p < 0.001). b Between-group comparison showing higher activation in the right cingulate gyrus/middle frontal gyrus and the left middle frontal gyrus in children with ASD compared with the ADHD group and in the right middle frontal gyrus in children with ASD compared with the TD group.

The ROI analysis revealed a significant effect after Bonferroni correction in the right posterior cingulate cortex in the ADHD group (pSVC-FEW < 0.005). No ROI survived Bonferroni correction within the ASD and the TD group.

Between-groups comparison

Whole-brain analyses: A one-way ANOVA analysis (Table 2 and Fig. 2b) revealed a significant group effect in the right cingulate/MFG at the cluster level pFWE < 0.05 (p = 0.030; cluster size = 152) and peak-level pFWE < 0.05 (p = 0.017; x = 16, y = 24, z = 42 mm). Pairwise comparisons showed that the group effect in this region was due to a significant higher activation in the ASD group during successful inhibition compared with the ADHD group (p = 0.002, peak-level pFWE < 0.05; Fig. 3a). Higher activation in the ASD group during successful inhibition compared with the ADHD group was also found in the left MFG at the cluster level pFWE < 0.05 (p = 0.018; x = −22; y = 16; z = 42; Fig. 3b). Finally, we found higher activation in the right MFG in the ASD group compared with the TD participants at the cluster (pFWE < 0.001; cluster size = 564) and peak level (pFWE = 0.020; x = 40, y = 20, z = 44 mm) during successful inhibition (Fig. 3c). Results for these analyses were comparable with or without age entered as a covariate. In an exploratory analysis, a one-way ANOVA with four groups was subsequently performed, in which the ASD sample was divided into two groups: non-comorbid ASD (n = 4) and ASD with ADHD comorbidity (n = 9). A small volume correction with a 10 mm sphere radius centred on the three coordinates reported above was examined at the peak-level pSVC-FWE < 0.05. Activations remained significant in all these regions, suggesting that between-group differences were not explained by the presence of comorbid ADHD in nine patients with ASD.

ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation? (3)

Percent signal change in activation during successful stop and go in regions showing between-group significant differences: a the right cingulate gyrus/ right middle frontal gyrus (x = 16, y = 24, z = 42), b the left middle frontal gyrus (x = −22, y = 16, z = 42) and c the right middle frontal gyrus (x = 40, y = 20, z = 44).

Finally, conjunction analysis did not show common activations between the patients groups.

ROI analyses: Analyses conducted on ROIs disclosed higher activation in the ADHD than the TD group in the right posterior cingulate gyrus (pSVC-FWE < 0.005; x = 4, y = −42, z = 20 mm) after Bonferroni correction. No other ROIs survived Bonferroni correction.

Discussion

In this study, both inhibition deficits and slower responding were found in the ADHD as compared with the TD group at the behavioural level, consistent with previous studies8,9. No differences in task performance were found between children with ASD and TD children, consistent with Schmitt et al.19, who found that the ability to reactively stop behaviours as measured by the SSRT was preserved in a group of children and adults with ASD. Nevertheless, in their study, RT slowing during go trials was reduced in patients compared with controls, suggesting a deficit in strategically delaying the onset of behavioural responses, i.e., proactive control.

fMRI analyses revealed different activation patterns in children with ASD and ADHD when explored separately, even if nine participants in the ASD group had an ADHD-like comorbidity. Conjunction analysis did not show common activations between the patients groups, emphasizing the between-group distinctiveness in activation patterns. In the ADHD group, successful inhibition activated a cluster comprising the right angular gyrus, the intraparietal sulcus and the inferior parietal gyrus, in line with previous studies in children and adolescents with ADHD40,41. These regions have been associated with action cancellation and action withholding42.

In children with ASD, activation was found mostly in frontal regions, in particular, the right MFG, also associated with action withholding and cancellation42. These results show that response inhibition may place different demands on the fronto-parietal network in ASD and ADHD, eventually activating regions that serve different functions in response inhibition. Although the inferior parietal cortex has been associated with rapid movement prevention43 and attentional capture of infrequent stimuli44, its contribution to inhibition per se remains unclear. The recruitment of parietal nodes in children with ADHD suggests they may need a higher involvement of attentional processes for successful inhibition than children with ASD, thereby modulating the response inhibition process.

Between-groups comparisons disclosed higher activation in the ASD than the ADHD group in the right cingulate gyrus/MFG and the left MFG. These results are consistent with Chantiluke et al.16, who also found increased activation in the left middle/IFC in boys with ASD as compared with those with ADHD, for a comparable task performance. In addition, there was also higher activation in the right MFG in the ASD group as compared with the TD group during successful inhibition. These results suggest that children with ASD may require recruiting more of these brain areas to achieve a task performance comparable to the ADHD and the TD groups. Differences observed in the left frontal cortex indicate that the left hemisphere should not be neglected in the study of response inhibition in ASD, as previously suggested45.

Regarding the ACC, ROI analysis revealed higher inhibition-related activity in the ASD than in the TDC and the ADHD groups, in agreement with previous studies evidencing atypical brain activity during response inhibition in autism45,46,47. However, this difference did not survive Bonferroni correction and the result should thus be considered with caution. The ACC has been associated with conflict and performance monitoring and salience detection during response inhibition48,49. Finally, ROI analysis revealed inhibition-related differences between the ADHD and TD groups in posterior cingulate activity, which is consistent with recent preliminary meta-analysis results50.

The findings in this study should be considered in the light of some limitations. First, our final sample was relatively small due to strict exclusion criteria regarding head motion and/or non-adequate SST performance. The strict but necessary exclusion task performance criteria were based on the latest consensus recommendations by Verbruggen et al.22 to analyse SST data in such a way that the ability to inhibit is accurately captured. Such precautions have often been dismissed or have been applied in a more lenient way in previous studies. Therefore, we argue that the strict exclusion criteria used actually represents the strength of our study, admittedly at the cost of a reduced sample size. A related limitation is that our sample was only composed of individuals able to perform the task inside of the scanner and whose data were included after data quality control regarding task performance and head motion. Consequently, the results of our study may not be generalizable to all individuals with ASD and/or ADHD, and especially those who display more severe ASD and/or ADHD symptoms or exhibit lower cognitive abilities. Our gender balanced sample was however large enough to detect robust significant brain activation differences between groups in our whole brain analysis, even after correction for multiple comparisons. In addition, all children with ADHD were non-comorbid and medication naïve in order to avoid drug-induced brain changes previously evidenced in other studies16 and only one participant with ASD had a previous history of psychostimulant intake. Therefore, there was no potential confounding effect of medication and/or comorbidity on inhibition-related activity in the ADHD sample. Again however, strict inclusion criteria may hinder the generalization of our results to the heterogeneous population of ADHD patients who present various comorbidities and/or medication status in clinical practice. Finally, the power of our statistical design was somehow limited by the use of short inter-stimuli intervals including jitters (leading to increased variance inflation factors) aimed at keeping the task duration as short and motivating as possible for our specific populations, whose inclusion in fMRI studies remain a challenge.

Finally, there was a high percentage of children in the ASD group, who had an ADHD comorbidity, which is consistent with recent comorbidity reports2,3, but potentially confounding our findings. Nevertheless, similar results were found regardless of ADHD comorbidity. The distinct patterns of activation between disorders suggest that ADHD-like symptoms in ASD are associated with a different pattern of activation compared with non-comorbid ADHD and raise the question of the distinctiveness between ADHD-like symptoms present in ASD and pure ADHD, which constitutes a crucial clinical insight in the study of the comorbidity between ASD and ADHD.

To conclude, a distinct pattern of activation during successful inhibition in a sample of children with ADHD and a sample of children with ASD was evidenced for the first time using the SST, despite the presence of ADHD-like symptoms in nine participants in the ASD group. Children with ADHD showed activation in inferior parietal regions and children with ASD in bilateral middle frontal regions during successful inhibition as compared with go trials. The ASD group showed higher activation in the middle frontal cortex compared with TD participants, whereas the ADHD showed higher activation than the TD group in the right posterior cingulate. Importantly, only the ADHD and not the ASD group showed behavioural inhibition deficits compared with the TD group, reinforcing the idea that reactive inhibition is one of the core deficits in ADHD, but not in ASD. Future work should explore the commonalities and distinctions in inhibition-related activity in a larger sample of children with ASD fractionating the sample in terms of the presence or absence of ADHD-like symptoms and a sample of children with ADHD, to examine the differences between the ADHD-like comorbidity in ASD and a primary diagnosis of ADHD.

References

  1. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th edn (American Psychiatric Association, Arlington, VA, 2013).

  2. Kaat, A., Gadow, K. & Lecavalier, L. Psychiatric symptom impairment in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 959–969 (2013).

    Article Google Scholar

  3. Salazar, F. et al. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders in preschool and elementary school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 2283–2294 (2015).

    Article Google Scholar

  4. Jensen, C. & Steinhausen, H. Comorbid mental disorders in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a large nationwide study. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 7, 27–38 (2014).

    Article Google Scholar

  5. Antshel, K. & Russo, N. Autism spectrum disorders and ADHD: overlapping phenomenology, diagnostic issues, and treatment considerations. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 21, 34 (2019).

  6. Morris, S. E. & Cuthbert, B. N. Research Domain Criteria: cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimensions of behavior. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 14, 29–37 (2012).

  7. Craig, F. et al. A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 12, 1191–1202 (2016).

  8. Lipszyc, J. & Schachar, R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: a meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 1064–1076 (2010).

    Article Google Scholar

  9. Pievsky, M. & McGrath, R. The neurocognitive profile of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review of meta-analyses. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 33, 143–157 (2017).

    Article Google Scholar

  10. Logan, G. & Cowan, W. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a theory of an act of control. Psychol. Rev. 91, 295–327 (1984).

    Article Google Scholar

  11. Geurts, H., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H. & Sergeant, J. How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 836–854 (2004).

    Article Google Scholar

  12. van Hulst, B. et al. Children with ADHD symptoms show deficits in reactive but not proactive inhibition, irrespective of their formal diagnosis. Psychol. Med. 48, 2515–2521 (2018).

    Article Google Scholar

  13. Lemon, J., Gargaro, B., Enticott, P. & Rinehart, N. Brief report: executive functioning in autism spectrum disorders: a gender comparison of response inhibition. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 352–356 (2010).

    Article Google Scholar

  14. Verté, S., Geurts, H., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J. & Sergeant, J. Executive functioning in children with autism and Tourette syndrome. Dev Psychopathol. 17, 415–445 (2005).

  15. Adams, N. & Jarrold, C. Inhibition in autism: children with autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1052–1063 (2011).

    Article Google Scholar

  16. Chantiluke, K. et al. Inverse fluoxetine effects on inhibitory brain activation in non-comorbid boys with ADHD and with ASD. Psychopharmacology 232, 2071–2082 (2014).

    Article Google Scholar

  17. Gooskens, B. et al. No evidence of differences in cognitive control in children with autism spectrum disorder or obsessive-compulsive disorder: An fMRI study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 36, 100602 (2019).

    Article Google Scholar

  18. Ozonoff, S. & Strayer, D. Inhibitory Function in Nonretarded Children with Autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 27, 59–77 (1997).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  19. Schmitt, L., White, S., Cook, E., Sweeney, J. & Mosconi, M. Cognitive mechanisms of inhibitory control deficits in autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59, 586–595 (2017).

    Article Google Scholar

  20. McCarthy, H., Skokauskas, N. & Frodl, T. Identifying a consistent pattern of neural function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 869–880 (2013).

    Article Google Scholar

  21. Massat, I. et al. Hyperactivity in motor response inhibition networks in unmedicated children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 19, 101–111 (2016).

    Article Google Scholar

  22. Verbruggen, F. et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. eLife. 8, e46323 (2019).

  23. Kaufman, J. et al. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 980–988 (1997).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  24. DuPaul, G., Power, T., Anastopoulos, A. & Reid, R. ADHD Rating Scale - IV (The Guilford Press, New York, 1998).

    Google Scholar

  25. Lord, C., Rutter, M. & Le Couteur, A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685 (1994).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  26. Lord, C. et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30, 205–223 (2000).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  27. Schopler, E., Reichler, R., DeVellis, R. & Daly, K. Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 10, 91–103 (1980).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  28. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 4th edn (PsychCorp, San Antonia, TX, 2003).

  29. Roid, G. H. & Miller, L. J. Leiter International Performance Scale-revised (Stoelting CoWoodale, IL, 1997).

  30. Logan, G. D. In: Dagenbach, D. & Carr, T. H. (eds). Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language. p. 189–239 (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, USA, 1994).

  31. Congdon, E. et al. Measurement and reliability of response inhibition. Front. Psychol. 3, 37 (2012).

  32. Friston, K. J., Ashburner, J., Kiebel, S. J., Nichols, T. E. & Penny, W. D. Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images (Academic Press, 2007).

  33. Mazaika, P., Hoeft, F., Glover, G. & Reiss, A. Methods and software for fMRI analysis of clinical subjects. Neuroimage 47, S58 (2009).

    Article Google Scholar

  34. Chevrier, A., Noseworthy, M. & Schachar, R. Dissociation of response inhibition and performance monitoring in the stop signal task using event-related fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1347–1358 (2007).

    Article Google Scholar

  35. Friston, K., Williams, S., Howard, R., Frackowiak, R. & Turner, R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 346–355 (1996).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  36. White, C. et al. Decomposing decision components in the stop-signal task: a model-based approach to individual differences in inhibitory control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1601–1614 (2014).

    Article Google Scholar

  37. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289 (2002).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  38. Gläscher, J. Visualization of group inference data in functional neuroimaging. Neuroinformatics 7, 73–82 (2009).

    Article Google Scholar

  39. Dennis, M. et al. Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 331–343 (2009).

    Article Google Scholar

  40. Bhaijiwala, M., Chevrier, A. & Schachar, R. Withholding and canceling a response in ADHD adolescents. Brain Behav. 4, 602–614 (2014).

    Article Google Scholar

  41. Rubia, K. et al. Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: a study with functional MRI. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 891–896 (1999).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  42. Zhang, R., Geng, X. & Lee, T. Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: an fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 3973–3990 (2017).

    Article Google Scholar

  43. Coxon, J., Stinear, C. & Byblow, W. Stop and Go: the neural basis of selective movement prevention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1193–1203 (2009).

    Article Google Scholar

  44. Boehler, C., Appelbaum, L., Krebs, R., Chen, L. & Woldorff, M. The role of stimulus salience and attentional capture across the neural hierarchy in a stop-signal task. PLoS ONE 6, e26386 (2011).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  45. Schmitz, N. et al. Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 7–16 (2006).

    Article Google Scholar

  46. Kana, R., Keller, T., Minshew, N. & Just, M. Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 198–206 (2007).

    Article Google Scholar

  47. Velasquez, F. et al. Neural correlates of emotional inhibitory control in autism spectrum disorders. Res Dev. Disabil. 64, 64–77 (2017).

    Article Google Scholar

  48. Braver, T. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb. Cortex. 11, 825–836 (2001).

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  49. Manza, P. et al. A dual but asymmetric role of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in response inhibition and switching from a non-salient to salient action. Neuroimage 134, 466–474 (2016).

    Article Google Scholar

  50. Norman, L. et al. F55. an image-based meta-analysis of successful and failed stopping in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder using statistical parametric maps. Biol. Psychiatry 85, S234 (2019).

    Article Google Scholar

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all the children and their families for their participation. I.M. has received a research grant from Shire Pharmaceutical Development Limited, a member of the Takeda group of companies (Study ID: IST-BEL-00520). A.A.S. is supported by a grant from the Belgian Kids’ Fund, the David et Alice Van Buuren Fund and by the Fondation Jaumotte-Demoulin. I.M. and A.A.S. are supported by the Fonds National de la Recherche Scientifique (FNRS)-Belgium and the Fonds Erasme. This publication was supported by the “Fondation Universitaire de Belgique”. We also thank Barbara Luyens and Clémence Roy for their assistance in the collection of the data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Neuropsychology and Functional Neuroimaging Research Group (UR2NF) at the Centre for Research in Cognition and Neurosciences (CRCN), Université Libre de Bruxelles (ULB), Brussels, Belgium

    Ariadna Albajara Sáenz,Simon Baijot,Leila Salvesen,Thomas Villemonteix,Philippe Peigneux&Isabelle Massat

  2. Hôpital Universitaire Robert Debré, Paris, France

    Mathilde Septier

  3. Institut de Psychiatrie et de Neurosciences de Paris Inserm U894 Team 1, Paris, France

    Mathilde Septier

  4. Department of Radiology, Universitair Ziekenhuis Brussel (UZ Brussel), Brussels, Belgium

    Peter Van Schuerbeek&Hubert Raeymaekers

  5. Hôpital Universitaire des Enfants Reine Fabiola (HUDERF), Brussels, Belgium

    Simon Baijot,Nicolas Deconinck,Véronique Delvenne&Gianfranco Passeri

  6. Service Universitaire Spécialisé pour personnes avec Autisme (SUSA)-Université de Mons, Mons, Belgium

    Pierre Defresne&Eric Willaye

  7. PsyPluriel, Centre Européen de Psychologie Médicale, Brussels, Belgium

    Laurent Victoor

  8. Paris 8 Vincennes - St Denis University, Laboratoire de Psychopathologie et Neuropsychologie, Saint Denis, France

    Thomas Villemonteix

  9. Laboratory of Experimental Neurology, ULB, Brussels, Belgium

    Isabelle Massat

  10. National Fund of Scientific Research (FNRS), Brussels, Belgium

    Isabelle Massat

  11. Department of Neurology, Erasme Hospital, Brussels, Belgium

    Isabelle Massat

Authors

  1. Ariadna Albajara Sáenz

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  2. Mathilde Septier

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  3. Peter Van Schuerbeek

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  4. Simon Baijot

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  5. Nicolas Deconinck

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  6. Pierre Defresne

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  7. Véronique Delvenne

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  8. Gianfranco Passeri

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  9. Hubert Raeymaekers

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  10. Leila Salvesen

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  11. Laurent Victoor

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  12. Thomas Villemonteix

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  13. Eric Willaye

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  14. Philippe Peigneux

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  15. Isabelle Massat

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Isabelle Massat.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation? (4)

Cite this article

Albajara Sáenz, A., Septier, M., Van Schuerbeek, P. et al. ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation?. Transl Psychiatry 10, 24 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0707-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0707-z

ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation? (2024)

FAQs

ADHD and ASD: distinct brain patterns of inhibition-related activation? ›

Distinct patterns of brain activity were observed during successful inhibition. In children with ADHD, motor inhibition was associated with right inferior parietal activation, whereas right frontal regions were activated in children with ASD.

What is the relationship between ADHD and ASD? ›

In fact, ADHD is the most common coexisting condition in children with ASD. On the flip side, up to a quarter of children with ADHD have low-level signs of ASD, which might include having difficulty with social skills or being very sensitive to clothing textures, for example.

How is the brain of autism and ADHD different? ›

Overall, autistic individuals had significantly greater cortical volume and thickness in the superior temporal gyrus; whereas individuals with ADHD had significant and widespread decreases in cortical volume and surface area, and increases in cortical thickness.

What is the link between ADHD and autism? ›

In a 2014 review of studies looking at the co-occurrence of ADHD and ASD, researchers found that between 30 to 50% of people with ASD also have symptoms of ADHD. Researchers don't fully understand the cause for either condition or why they occur together so frequently. Both conditions may be linked to genetics.

What do ASD and ADHD look like together? ›

Common experiences for ADHD and autism include sensory differences, intense focus on specific interests, rejection sensitivity, executive dysfunction, sleep issues and emotional dysregulation.

Why do people compare ADHD and autism? ›

While the symptoms of ADHD and autism may not look the same on paper, they can look very similar in person. Traits like distractibility and impulsivity, for example, are part of the ADHD diagnosis. While they're not part of the autism diagnosis, they appear in most people with autism.

What part of the brain is affected by ADHD and autism? ›

Taken together, these studies indicate that the cerebellum and corpus callosum are affected in similar ways in ASD and ADHD, while findings on total brain, prefrontal cortex, and basal ganglia volumes have shown opposite patterns in the two diseases.

What is the difference between ADHD brain and normal brain? ›

In sum. The ADHD brain is smaller than the non-ADHD brain and has fewer connections between different brain regions. Their brains do not have the neural organization to self-regulate and to stop automatic responses.

What are the comorbidities of ADHD and ASD? ›

70% of children with ASD will have at least one co-occurring psychiatric condition, and 41% will have two or more, the most common being social anxiety, ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder. Intellectual disability is one of the most common comorbidities of ADHD and ASD, 46% and up to 70%, respectively.

Are ADHD and autism hereditary? ›

ADHD and ASD are among the most common neurodevelopmental disorders in children and often persist throughout adulthood1. ADHD and ASD are both highly heritable (60–93%)2,3,4, and the mode of their inheritance is complex and polygenic.

What is stimming? ›

Stimming refers to repetitive behaviors or motions that you may use to help cope with emotions. Some examples of stimming include: Biting your nails when you feel anxious. Twirling your hair when you feel bored. Flapping your hands when something excites you‌

What is auditory stimming? ›

Auditory stimming uses the person's sense of hearing and sound. It may include behaviors such as: vocal sounds, such as humming. tapping on objects or ears, covering and uncovering ears, and finger-snapping. repetitive speech.

What does high functioning autism look like? ›

Symptoms of High-Functioning Autism

Some common characteristics of HFA include: Difficulty with social communication and interaction: Individuals with HFA may struggle with understanding social cues, making eye contact, and engaging in reciprocal conversations.

What is stimming ADHD? ›

What Is ADHD Stimming? ADHD stimming is when a person with ADHD displays self-stimulatory behavior by repeating certain sounds and movements unconsciously. There are many different examples, including lip biting, rocking back and forth, humming, teeth grinding, or chewing gum.

How do autistic brains think differently? ›

How Does An Autistic Brain Think? Another aspect of the autistic brain is its unique way of thinking. People with ASD often have a strong focus on details and patterns, which can make them excellent problem solvers, particularly in fields such as mathematics, science or engineering.

Are autistic brains actually different? ›

The study identified 194 significantly different genes in the brains of people with autism. Of those genes, 143 produced more mRNA (upregulated) and 51 produced less (downregulated) in autistic brains than in typical ones. The downregulated genes were mainly linked to brain connectivity.

What makes an autistic brain different? ›

Specifically, in autistic brains there is significantly more folding in the left parietal and temporal lobes as well as in the right frontal and temporal regions. “These alterations are often correlated with modifications in neuronal network connectivity,” Culotta says.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Foster Heidenreich CPA

Last Updated:

Views: 5982

Rating: 4.6 / 5 (76 voted)

Reviews: 91% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Foster Heidenreich CPA

Birthday: 1995-01-14

Address: 55021 Usha Garden, North Larisa, DE 19209

Phone: +6812240846623

Job: Corporate Healthcare Strategist

Hobby: Singing, Listening to music, Rafting, LARPing, Gardening, Quilting, Rappelling

Introduction: My name is Foster Heidenreich CPA, I am a delightful, quaint, glorious, quaint, faithful, enchanting, fine person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.